, AT-Kubark Pt 2--Price, Wikileaks, Compleat Wikileaks Archive, Misc 

AT-Kubark Pt 2--Price

AT-Kubark Pt 2--Price, Wikileaks, Compleat Wikileaks Archive, Misc
[ Pobierz całość w formacie PDF ]
Buying a piece of anthropology
Part Two: The CIA and our tortured past
DaviD H. Price
David H. Price is associate
professor of anthropology
at Saint Martin’s University.
He is author of
Threatening
anthropology: McCarthyism
and the FBI’s surveillance
of activist anthropologists
(2004) and the forthcoming
Anthropological intelligence:
The deployment and neglect
of American anthropology
during the Second World
War
(Duke University Press,
2008).
His email is dprice@
stmartin.edu.
This is the second part of a two-part article by David Price
examining how research on stress under Human Ecology
Fund sponsorship found its way into the CIA’s
Kubark
interrogation manual (for Part 1 see our June issue). This
issue of
ANTHROPOLOGY TODAY
also features a short com-
ment by Roberto González on the use of Ralph Patai’s
The Arab mind
in training interrogators who worked in
Iraq, including at Abu Ghraib (p. 23). See also news, p.
28, for a pledge initiated by the Network of Concerned
Anthropologists in response to anthropologists’ concerns
around this issue.
[
Editor
]
Back in 1994, my curiosity concerning interactions
between anthropologists and the Human Ecology Fund
(HEF) was raised when I found published announcements
of anthropologists receiving HEF funds in old newsletters
of the American Anthropological Association (AAA).
1
One
article listed nine HEF grant recipients: Preston S. Abbott,
William K. Carr, Janet A. Hartle, Alan Howard, Barnaby
C. Keeney, Raymond Prince, Robert A. Scott, Leon Stover
and Robert C. Suggs (FN, 1966[2]). I tried to contact each
scholar, and Howard, Scott and Stover replied to my initial
inquiries about their HEF-sponsored research.
2
In late 1994 I wrote to Alan Howard and Robert A.
Scott, asking what they remembered about the Fund, their
research and if they knew of the Fund’s connection to the
CIA. When I emailed Howard at the University of Hawaii,
asking him what he knew about the CIA’s covert funding
of their research, Howard expressed anguished surprise,
replying, ‘Agh! I had no idea’ (AH to DHP 11/2/94).
Howard had remained in contact with Robert Scott, to
whom he had forwarded my correspondence. Scott later
wrote me a letter detailing how he came to receive the
funds:
[I] had absolutely no idea that the Human Ecology Fund was a
front for anything, least of all the CIA. As far as I knew it was
a small fund that was controlled by Harold Wolff and used to
support projects of various types concerning the study of stress
and illness in humans. Its connection with the CIA only came
to my attention some years later when Jay Schulman… wrote
an article exposing the connection.
3
Obviously if I had known
of such a connection at the time I would never have accepted
money from them. I should also explain that the money we got
from them was used to support library research I was doing at
the Cornell Medical School on studies of stress and that the
final product was a theoretical model for the study of stress
in humans.
I will explain how I came to know about the Fund in the first
place. The period of time would have been roughly from 1961-
1963. I finished my doctorate in sociology at Stanford University
in 1960 and then received a two-year post-doctoral fellowship
in medical sociology from the Russell Sage Foundation. I spent
the first year at Stanford Medical School and then moved on to
the Cornell Medical School for a second year of work… I was
interested in studying stress and illness and the work of Harold
Wolff, his colleague Larry Hinkle and others was far closer to
the mark. I therefore arranged to transfer my post-doc to a unit
headed by Hinkle and with which Harold Wolff had an affili-
ation. The name of that unit was The Human Ecology Studies
Program. At the time I was there, Larry Hinkle was completing
a study of stress among telephone operators working for New
Jersey (or was it New York) Bell Telephone company and he
was also beginning a study of stress and heart disease among
a group of executives for the New Jersey Bell Company. He
invited me to participate in the analysis for the first study and
to advise him about the design of several of the instruments
used in connection with that project. At the same time, I was
also working with Alan [Howard] on an article about stress
and it was in connection with this work that I received sup-
port from the Fund. Or at least I think that is the reason why
I acknowledged the Fund in our paper… I do remember that
Fig. 1. Allen Dulles (1893-
1969), who authorized
MK-Ultra as CIA Director of
Central Intelligence.
MK-Ultra
Headed by Dr Sidney
Gottlieb, the CIA’s MK-Ultra
project was set up in the early
1950s largely in response to
alleged Soviet, Chinese and
North Korean use of mind-
control techniques on US
prisoners of war in Korea.
The project involved covert
research at an estimated 30
universities and institutions
in an extensive programme
of experimentation that
included chemical, biological
and radiological tests,
often on unwitting citizens.
It was not until the 1970s
that this programme was
exposed, but by that time
many scholars from a wide
range of disciplines had been
implicated.
either Hinkle or Wolff or both suggested that I write a letter to
the Fund requesting a modest level of support for our work (I
can’t remember the amount, but I am reasonably certain it came
to no more than a few thousand dollars)…
It will be obvious to you from reading this that I knew Harold
Wolff for a brief period of time during this period. As I recall,
Wolff [died] either in 1962 or 1963. From the manner in which
the matter was handled I gained the impression that he had
available to him a small fund of money that could be used to
support research and writing of the sort I was doing and he gave
me some for my work. At that time there were lots of small
pots of money sitting around medical school and there was no
reason to be suspicious about this one. Moreover, Wolff was a
figure of great distinction in neurology and was well known
outside of his field as well. For all of these reasons I simply
assumed that everything was completely legitimate and was
astounded when the connection between the Fund and the CIA
was disclosed.
… I should also mention that during the course of our col-
laboration Alan [Howard] and I co-authored a second paper
on cultural variations in conceptions of death and dying which
was also published and in which there is an acknowledgment
to the Fund.
4
[…] My association with the Human Ecology Studies Program
came to an end early in 1964. In September of 1963 I left
the program to become a Research Associate on the staff of
Russell Sage Foundation in order to conduct a study they had
just funded. As I recall, for a short while during the fall of
1963 I [spent] a small amount of time at the Human Ecology
Study Program advising project members about various issues
involving their research on heart disease, but this eventually
fell by the way side as I became more deeply drawn into the
new project. (RAS to DHP 11/2/94)
At the time both Howard and Scott were unaware that
the research funds they received came from the CIA. Their
accounts of their interactions with HEF make sense, given
This paper benefited from
comments by Alexander
Cockburn, Alan Howard,
Robert Lawless, Steve Niva,
Eric Ross, Robert Scott,
Jeffrey St. Clair and three
anonymous AT reviewers.
1. One
Fellow Newsletter
article announced that
William Carr had ‘joined the
staff of the Human Ecology
Fund in March’, and that
the Fund contributed to the
financing of Raymond Prince
and Francis Speed’s film
Were
ni! He is a madman
, which
documented the treatment of
Yoruba mental disorders (FN
1964[5]: 6). The May 1962
issue of the
Newsletter
invited
anthropologists to apply for
funds.
2. Leon Stover wrote that
his HEF grant was arranged
by ‘a close friend who worked
ANTHROPOLOGY TODAY VOL 23 NO 5, OcTObeR 2007
17
Fig. 2. Table summarizing the
results of a study comparing
distress ratings of 300 torture
victims from Yugoslavia,
comparing psychological with
physical torture. (See Khamsi,
Roxanne. Psychological
torture ‘as bad as physical
torture’.
New Scientist
, 5
March 2007.)
how Wolff and Hinkle shielded participants from any knowl-
edge of CIA involvement or of the MK-Ultra project.
In 1998 I published an article briefly describing MK-
Ultra’s use of the HEF to channel CIA funds to anthro-
pologists and other social scientists, but as the
Kubark
counterintelligence interrogation
manual had not yet
been declassified, I did not mention or connect Scott
and Howard’s research with MK-Ultra’s objective of
researching effective models of interrogation (Price 1998;
for more on MK-Ultra, see Part 1 of this article). It was
not until I read Alfred McCoy’s book
A question of tor-
ture
(2006) that I noticed the relevance of their research on
stress for
Kubark
. Until then I had assumed that their work
was funded to reinforce an air of (false) legitimacy for the
HEF – much as I interpreted the funding of anthropolo-
gist Janet Hartel’s study of the Smithsonian’s Mongolian
skull collection. However, McCoy clarifies that research
on stress was vital to MK-Ultra (e.g., McCoy 2006), and
HEF-sponsored research projects selectively harvested
research that went into design of effective ‘coercive inter-
rogation’ techniques.
5
[T]he CIA distilled its findings in its seminal
Kubark
Counterinsurgency Interrogation
handbook. For the next forty
years, the
Kubark
manual would define the agency’s interroga-
tion methods and training program throughout the Third World.
Synthesizing the behavioral research done by contract aca-
demics, the manual spelled out a revolutionary two-phase form
of torture that relied on sensory deprivation and self-inflicted
pain for an effect that, for the first time in the two millennia
of their cruel science, was more psychological than physical.
(McCoy 2006: 50)
for the Fund’, but after I sent
him further documentation on
the CIA’s role in funding his
research, he did not respond
(LS to DP 11/28/94).
3. Sociologist Jay
Schulman was part of Human
Ecology’s programme
studying Hungarian refugees
(Greenfield 1977, Stephenson
1978, US Senate 1977).
4. Another HEF-sponsored
research project undertaken
by Howard funded the
organization of data collected
while conducting fieldwork
on Rotuman sexuality
(Howard & Howard 1964).
Howard later co-authored a
paper (with no connection to
HEF) examining symbolic
and functional features of
torture traditionally practised
by the Huron on prisoners-of-
war and other cultural groups
(Bilmes & Howard 1980).
5. McCoy speculates that
Stanley Milgram’s research
was covertly CIA funded
under such programmes,
but Milgram’s biographer
disputes even the possibility
that Milgram was unwittingly
funded (cf. McCoy 2006,
Blass 2006).
6. DCI Stansfield Turner
mistakenly testified that the
Privacy Act prevented the
identification of scholars
working on MK-Ultra
projects at Human Ecology
(US Senate 1977). Harold
Wolff was dead and thus had
no rights under the Privacy
Act.
7. History repeats itself,
as US interrogators recently
drew on their torture
resistance training to develop
abusive techniques with data
from the SERE programme
(DoD 2006, Soldz 2007b).
8. Kleinman’s
consideration of
Kubark’s
fundamental philosophical
approach to interrogation
summarized
Kubark’s
paradigms as relying on:
psychological assessment,
screening, the creation and
release of controlled stress,
isolation and regression,
which are all used by
interrogators to ‘help’
the interrogation subject
‘concede’ (Kleinman 2006).
9. Marks described a 1958
HEF grant studying inner-
city youth gang members in
which sociologist Muzafer
Sherif had no idea that the
CIA funded the project
to model how to manage
KGB defectors. An MK-
Ultra source told Marks the
CIA learned that ‘getting a
juvenile delinquent [gang]
defector was motivationally
mid-1950s, Hinkle and Wolff also studied the role of con-
trolled stress in ‘breaking’ and ‘brainwashing’ prisoners of
war and communist enemies of state. They became experts
on coercive interrogation and published their study on
‘Communist interrogation and indoctrination of “enemies
of the state” in Communist countries’ (1956). But they
also produced a ‘classified secret’ version of this paper for
CIA DCI Allen Dulles (Rév 2002). Whilst passing secret
reports along to the CIA, Wolff produced HEF-funded
public research publications studying interrogation, such
as his 1960 publication ‘Every man has his breaking point:
The conduct of prisoners of war’ (see also HEF 1963).
MK-Ultra funds encouraged scholars to contribute to
their study of brainwashing and coercive interrogation,
supposedly benefiting military and intelligence branches
by helping them to train spies and troops to better resist
interrogation techniques. Later, this research was secretly
used in the production of the
Kubark
manual, which became
less a guide to resisting interrogation than an interroga-
tion manual to be used against enemies – with some forms
of coercion that violated the Geneva Convention.
7
Such
dual purpose became a recurrent practice in the work of
scholars operating within MK-Ultra’s shrouded network.
While studies by Wolff and Hinkle and other HEF-
funded scholars had medical implications, their work also
had practical relevance for CIA interrogation techniques.
Wolff and Hinkle established research of interest to
Kubark
by establishing a research milieu at HEF whilst keeping
their connections to the MK-Ultra programme well hidden.
In the early 1960s independent scholars undertook their
own work and shared ideas with others working in similar
areas, resulting in cross-pollination of ideas.
Though it remains unclear exactly how independent
academic models of stress were worked into MK-Ultra’s
objectives, continuities are evident between Howard and
Scott’s 1965 stress article and
Kubark’s
guiding para-
digms.
8
John Marks claims that the HEF ‘
put money into
projects whose covert application was so unlikely that
only an expert could see the possibilities
’ (Marks 1979:
170; my italics).
9
McCoy argues that the CIA funded HEF
projects to gather information, encouraged by Wolff or by
CIA officers involved in the
Kubark
manual. A declassified
1963 internal CIA memo stated that ‘a substantial portion
of the MKULTRA record appears to rest in the memories
of the principal officers’ (CIA 1963a: 23), so it seems HEF
findings were mostly incorporated informally.
Because the CIA destroyed most of its MK-Ultra records
in 1972 (Marks 1979), we do not know who drafted
Wolff, Hinkle, HEF, MK-Ultra and
Kubark
The US Senate’s 1977 hearings investigating MK-Ultra’s
co-optation of academic research did not identify the indi-
vidual academics who co-ordinated HEF’s research for the
CIA. Senator Edward Kennedy interrupted CIA psycholo-
gist John Gittinger’s testimony as he was about to identify
HEF staff cognizant of CIA secret sponsorship of academic
research. Kennedy told Gittinger that the committee was
‘not interested in names or institutions, so we prefer that
you do not. That has to be worked out in arrangements
between [Director of Central Intelligence] Admiral Turner
and the individuals and the institutions’ (US Senate 1977:
59).
6
John Marks first documented how cardiologist Lawrence
E. Hinkle, Jr and neurologist Harold G. Wolff became the
heart and mind of Human Ecology’s CIA enquiries. Hinkle
and Wolff were both professors at Cornell University’s
Medical School, and after CIA Director Allen Dulles asked
Wolff to review what was known of ‘brainwashing’ tech-
niques, a partnership developed in which ‘Hinkle handled
the administrative part of the study and shared in the sub-
stance [of research]’ (Marks 1979: 135).
A respected neurologist who specialized in migraines
and other forms of headache pain (Blau 2004), Wolff
had experimentally induced and measured headaches
in research subjects at Cornell since as far back as 1935
(SN 1935). Hinkle conducted research at Cornell from
the 1950s until his retirement (AMWS 2005, vol. 3); his
early career focused on environmental impacts on cardio-
vascular health. Together, Hinkle and Wolff studied ‘the
mechanisms by which the individual man adapts to his
particular environment, and the effect of these adaptations
upon his disease’ (Hinkle 1965). Wolff died in 1962, a
year before the CIA produced its
Kubark
manual; Hinkle
remained at Cornell for decades, later retiring to the com-
forts of suburban Connecticut.
Hinkle and Wolff pioneered studies of workplace stress,
effects of stress on cardiovascular health and migraines
that brought legitimacy and helped make HEF grant recip-
ients keen to collaborate (Hinkle and Wolff 1957). By the
18
ANTHROPOLOGY TODAY VOL 23 NO 5, OcTObeR 2007
Fig. 3. The 1953 CIA memo
from DCI Allen Dulles
authorizing MK-Ultra.
Kubark
or the details of how HEF research made its way
into the manual. However,
Kubark’s
reliance on citations
from HEF-funded research, and testimony at the 1977
Senate hearings stating that MK-Ultra research was used to
develop interrogation and resistance methods, demonstrate
that HEF research was incorporated (US Senate 1977).
The 1977 Senate hearings on MK-Ultra programmes
detailed the CIA’s failures to find esoteric means of using
hypnosis, psychedelics, ‘truth serums’, sensory depriva-
tion tanks or electroshock to interrogate unco-operative
subjects. John Gittinger testified that by 1963, after years
of experimentation, the CIA realized that ‘brainwashing
was largely a process of isolating a human being, keeping
him out of contact, putting him under long stress in rela-
tionship to interviewing and interrogation, and that they
could produce any change that way without having to
resort to any kind of esoteric means’ (US Senate 1977: 62).
With
isolation
and
stress
having become the magic bullets
for effective coercive interrogation, it was in the context
of this shift away from drugs and equipment that Human
Ecology sponsored Howard and Scott’s stress research.
The ‘coercive interrogation’ techniques
Kubark
described
shade into torture by the application of intense stress or
isolation in order to induce confessions.
Because
Kubark
was an instruction manual, not an aca-
demic treatise, no authors are identified. Although a few
academic sources are cited, most sources remain unac-
knowledged. HEF-sponsored work cited included: Martin
Orne’s hypnosis research, Biderman and Zimmer’s work
on non-voluntary behaviour, Hinkle’s work on pain and
the physiological state of interrogation subjects, John
Lilly’s sensory deprivation research, and Karla Roman’s
graphology research (CIA 1963b).
Kubark
discussed the importance of interrogators
learning to read the body language of interrogation sub-
jects, which the HEF-funded anthropologist Edward Hall
pursued. Several pages of
Kubark
describe how to read
subject’s body language with tips such as:
It is also helpful to watch the subject’s mouth, which is as a rule
much more revealing than his eyes. Gestures and postures also
tell a story. If a subject normally gesticulates broadly at times
and is at other times physically relaxed but at some point sits
stiffly motionless, his posture is likely to be the physical image
of his mental tension. The interrogator should make a mental
note of the topic that caused such a reaction. (CIA 1963b: 55)
not all that much different
from getting a Soviet one’
(Marks 1959: 159; cf. HEF
1963).
10. Hall’s previous work
in
The silent language
discussed the role played by
cultural expectations in the
interrogation of Japanese
prisoners in the Second World
War (Hall 1959).
11. Marvin Opler arranged
Barbara Anderson’s HEF
support (Anderson 1965).
12. Howard recalls that
although the paper was
submitted in 1961 it was not
published until 1965, owing
to delays caused by the death
of Franz Alexander, one of
the paper’s peer reviewers
(AH to DHP 6/5/07).
13. Prohibitions that were
enacted in the 1970s after
knowledge of MK-Ultra,
COINTELPRO and other
unregulated intelligence
programmes became known
to the public and Congress.
AAA 2007. ‘Update: AAA
Adopts Resolutions on
Iraq and Torture, 7 June
www.aaanet.org/press/
PR20061211.htm
Anderson, Barbara Gallatin
1965. Bereavement as a
subject of cross-cultural
inquiry.
Anthropological
Quarterly
38(4): 181-200.
AMWS 2005.
American men
and women of science
,
23rd edition. New
Providence, NJ: Bowker.
American Psychological
Association [APA] 2007.
‘American Psychological
Association calls on US
government to prohibit
the use of unethical
interrogation techniques’.
www.apa.org/releases/
councilres0807.html
Blass, Thomas 2006.
‘Milgram and the CIA
stanleymilgram.com/
rebuttal.php; accessed
2/8/07.
Blau, J.N. 2004. Harold G.
Wolff: The man and his
migraine.
Cephalalgia
24(3): 215-222.
Bilmes, Jacob and Howard,
Alan 1980. Pain as cultural
drama.
Anthropology and
Humanism
5(2-3): 10-13.
CIA 1963a. MKULTRA
document labelled:
‘Report of inspection
of MKULTRA/TSD’ 1-
185209, cy 2 See D, 26
July [declassified].
— 1963b.
Kubark
counterintelligence
interrogation
[manual]
[declassified].
— 1983.
Human resource
exploitation training
manual
[declassified].
In 1977, after public revelations of the CIA’s role in
directing HEF research projects, Edward Hall discussed
his unwitting receipt of CIA funds through the HEF to sup-
port his writing of
The hidden dimension
(Hall 1966). Hall
conceded that his studies of body language would have
been useful for the CIA’s goals, ‘because the whole thing
is designed to begin to teach people to understand, to read
other people’s behavior. What little I know about the [CIA],
I wouldn’t want to have much to do with it’ (Greenfield
1977: 11).
10
But Hall’s work, like that of others, entered
Human Ecology’s knowledge base, which was selectively
drawn upon for
Kubark
.
The HEF provided travel grants for anthropologist
Marvin Opler and an American delegation attending the
1964 First International Congress of Social Psychiatry in
London. The Wenner-Gren Foundation also provided funds
for a ‘project in the Cross-Cultural Study of Psychoactive
Drugs which was presented at the Congress’, where Opler
presented a paper under that title (Opler 1965).
Though not known to be funded by HEF, Mark
Zborowksi established a position at Cornell with Wolff’s
assistance, where he conducted research for his book
examining the cultural mitigation of pain,
People in pain
(Zborowski 1969, Encandela 1993).
Kubark’s
approach to
pain referenced Hinkle and Wolff, and incorporated many
of Zborowski’s ideas. Anthropologist Rhoda Métraux
assisted Wolff and Hinkle’s research into the impact
of stress among Chinese individuals unable to return to
China (Hinkle et al. 1957). When Wolff learned that Rhoda
Métraux would not be granted research clearance by the
CIA, he lied to her about the nature of their work (Marks
1979). Hinkle later admitted that this HEF project’s secret
goal was to recruit skilled CIA intelligence operatives
who could return to China as spies. Métraux’s unwitting
participation helped collect information later used by the
CIA to train agents to resist Chinese forms of interrogation
(Marks 1979).
It is not clear why the HEF sponsored anthropological
research on grieving; perhaps they recognized in bereave-
ment a universal experience of intense stress and isolation
mitigated by culture, or perhaps the CIA was interested
in studying the impact of mourning on POWs coping
with the loss of fellow soldiers. Medical anthropologist
Barbara Anderson received HEF funds to write an article
on ‘bereavement as a subject of cross-cultural inquiry’ (see
Anderson 1965).
11
Though HEF only funded the write-up
of their stress article, Alan Howard and Robert Scott also
ANTHROPOLOGY TODAY VOL 23 NO 5, OcTObeR 2007
19
Cockburn, Alexander and
St. Clair, Jeffrey 1998.
Whiteout
. London: Verso.
Democracy Now
2007.
‘The task force report
should be annulled.’
www,democracynow.
org/article.
pl?sid=07/06/01/1457247.
Department of Defense, US
[DoD] 2006. ‘Review
of DoD-directed
investigations of detainee
irp/agency/dod/abuse.pdf
(accessed 29/05/07).
Fair, Eric 2007. An Iraq
interrogator’s nightmare.
Washington Post
9
February: A19.
Fellow Newsletter
, American
Anthropological
Association [FN] passim.
Encandela, John A. 1993.
Social science and the
study of pain since
Zborowski: A need for
a new agenda.
Social
Science and Medicine
36(6): 783-791.
Gordon, Nathan J. and
Fleisher, William L. 2006.
Effective interviewing and
interrogation techniques,
2nd ed. Amsterdam:
Elsevier.
Greenfield, Patricia 1977.
CIA’s behavior caper.
APA Monitor
December:
1, 10-11.
Gross, Terry 2007.
‘Scott Shane on US
interrogation techniques.
WHYY’s Fresh Air 6
org/templates/story/story.
php?storyId=10763378
Hall, Edward T. 1966.
The
hidden dimension
. Garden
City: Doubleday.
— 1959.
The silent language
.
Greenwich, Conn:
Fawcett.
Hinkle, Lawrence 1961. The
physiological state of the
interrogation subject as
it affects brain function.
In: Biderman, A.D. and
Zimmer, H. (eds)
The
manipulation of human
behavior
, pp. 19-50. New
York: John Wiley & Sons.
— 1965. Division of Human
Ecology, Cornell Medical
Center.
BioScience
15(8):
532.
— et al. 1957. Studies in
human ecology: Factors
governing the adaptation
of Chinese unable to return
to China. In: Hoch, Paul
H. and Zubin, Joseph
(eds)
Experimental
psychopathology
, pp. 170-
186. New York: Grune &
Straton, Inc.
— and Wolff, H.G. 1956.
Communist interrogation
and indoctrination of
“enemies of the state”:
Analysis of methods used
by the Communist state
police.
AMA Archives of
Neurology and Psychiatry
76: 115.
— 1957. The nature of
man’s adaptation to his
total environment and the
relation of this to illness.
AMA Archives of Internal
produced an article entitled ‘cultural values and attitudes
toward death’ (Howard and Scott 1965/66). Although the
authors acknowledge HEF for making their collaboration
possible they stress that they did not notify the HEF of this
paper. Like the stress article, this paper was chiefly based
on Howard’s research into bereavement in Rotuma, which
was sponsored by the National Institute of Mental Health
(NIMH) (see Howard and Scott rejoinder below). This
focus on the way grief produces isolation and alienation
aligned with HEF’s broader interests and fit into
Kubark’s
interest in regression and psychic collapse.
Howard and Scott investigated the impact of encultura-
tion on the grieving process. They recognized that cultural
norms and behavioural practices shaped experiences of iso-
lation which, in turn, created different conditions of stress for
grieving individuals. The first half of their article examined
American ways of death, grieving and alienation, drawing on
Scott’s sociological perspective, while the second half used
Howard’s ethnographic knowledge to examine Rotuman
Polynesian attitudes to death, how they are socialized to
experience isolation differently and how these differences
translated to different cultural reactions to death.
The article cited environmental factors in stress from
Wolff, Hinkle and the HEF research, and drew upon
Kubzansky’s chapter on ‘the effects of reduced envi-
ronmental stimulation on human behavior’ in Biderman
and Zimmer’s HEF volume
The manipulation of human
behavior
– the source most heavily cited in
Kubark
(Howard
and Scott 1965/66). Out of the vast universe of writings on
death and bereavement, Howard and Scott’s selection of
this prison study illustrates how Human Ecology’s envi-
ronment influenced its sponsored studies. There is nothing
sinister or improper in their citation of these studies, but
their selection shows how HEF’s network of scholars
informed the production of knowledge. Some of Howard
and Scott’s views of isolation reflected HEF’s focus on the
isolation and vulnerability of prisoners:
While a fear of death may stem from anxieties about social
isolation, it seems equally true that the process of becoming
socially isolated stimulates a concern about death…When
social isolation is involuntary… the individual experiencing
separating from others may become obsessed with the idea of
death. (Howard and Scott 1965/66: 164)
For CIA sponsors looking over these academics’ shoul-
ders, death and bereavement formed part of a broader the-
matic focus on isolation and vulnerability.
determining the success of techniques of ‘coercive inter-
rogation’. The two authors worked together on this model
even before they heard of the HEF, and both claim they
would have undertaken the work even without HEF’s
funding (RS to DP 6/11/07). The HEF’s half-yearly report
described Howard and Scott’s research as developing an
‘equilibrium model… based upon a view of man as a
“problem solving” organism continually confronted with
situations requiring resolution to avoid stress and to pre-
serve well-being’ (HEF 1963: 24). In the world of aca-
demic scholarship this was innovative research; but from
the perspective of the CIA, ‘avoiding stress’ took on dif-
ferent meanings.
Howard and Scott’s 1965 article on stress was ‘reverse
engineered’ for information on how to weaken a subject’s
efforts to adapt to the stresses of interrogation. Thus, when
they wrote that ‘stress occurs if the individual does not have
available to him the tools and knowledge to either suc-
cessfully deal with or avert challenges which arise in par-
ticular situations,’ they were simultaneously scientifically
describing the factors mitigating the experience of stress
(
their
purpose), while also unwittingly outlining what envi-
ronmental factors should be manipulated if one wanted to
keep an individual under stressful conditions (
their hidden
CIA patron’s
purpose) (Howard and Scott 1965: 143).
Their 1965 article reviewed literature on how stress
interfered with gastric functions, and could cause or
increase frequency or severity of disease. They described
how individuals cope with stressful situations through
efforts to ‘maintain equilibrium in the face of difficult,
and in some cases almost intolerable circumstances’ (ibid.:
142). The research cited in their work included studies of
human reactions to stressful situations such as bombing
raids, impending surgery and student examinations.
Howard and Scott’s innovative ‘problem-solving’ model
for conceptualizing stress began with the recognition that
individuals under stress act to try and reduce their stress
and return to a state of equilibrium. The model posited that
‘disequilibrium motivates the organism to attempt to solve
the problems which produce the imbalance, and hence to
engage in problem-solving activity’ (ibid.: 145).
Under coercive interrogation, subjects would be
expected to try and reduce the ‘imbalance’ of discomfort
or pain and return to a state of equilibrium by providing the
interrogator with the requested information. Their model
could be adapted to view co-operation and question-
answering as the solution to the stressful problem faced
by interrogation subjects, so that rational subjects would
co-operate in order to return to their non-coercive state of
equilibrium. This philosophy aligned with a basic
Kubark
paradigm that
The effectiveness of most of the non-coercive techniques
depends upon their unsettling effect… The aim is to enhance
this effect, to disrupt radically the familiar emotional and
psychological associations of the subject. When this aim is
achieved, resistance is seriously impaired. There is an interval
– which may be extremely brief – of suspended animation, a
kind of psychological shock or paralysis. It is caused by a trau-
matic or sub-traumatic experience which explodes, as it were,
the world that is familiar to the subject as well as his image of
himself within that world. Experienced interrogators recognize
this effect when it appears and know that at this moment the
source is far more open to suggestion, far likelier to comply,
than he was just before he experienced the shock. (CIA 1963b:
65-66)
Thus a skilled interrogator ‘helps’ subjects move towards
‘compliance’, after which subjects may return to a desired
state of equilibrium.
Howard and Scott found that individuals under stress
had only three response options. They could mount an
‘assertive response’, in which they confronted the problem
directly and enacted a solution by mobilizing whatever
Stress models and the culture of
Kubark
research
Howard and Scott’s HEF grant supported their library
research and their writing-up. Scott was based at Cornell,
where he had contact with Hinkle, Wolff and other HEF per-
sonnel, while Howard wrote in California and never visited
Cornell. Prior to 1961 they submitted a copy of their HEF-
sponsored paper developing a ‘proposed framework for
the analysis of stress in the human organism’ to the journal
Behavioral Science
, and following normal procedures,
a copy of the paper was submitted to their funders (RS to
DP 6/11/07, Howard and Scott 1965).
12
In his 1977 Senate
testimony, Gettinger described how CIA funding of Human
Ecology allowed it to be ‘run exactly like any other founda-
tion’, which included having ‘access to any of the reports
that they had put out, but there were no strings attached to
anybody. There wasn’t any reason they couldn’t publish any-
thing that they put out’ (US Senate 1977: 59). Beyond what-
ever ‘normal’ conversations or ‘friendly’ suggestions there
might be, this was the principal way that the HEF research
findings were channelled to the CIA, who then selectively
harvested what they wanted for their own ends.
Scott and Howard’s work fit Wolff’s larger (public) pro-
gramme of studying stress and health, as well as Wolff’s
(both public and secret) programme studying the dynamics
20
ANTHROPOLOGY TODAY VOL 23 NO 5, OcTObeR 2007
Medicine
99: 442-460.
Howard, Alan and Howard,
Irwin 1964. Pre-marital
sex and social control
among the Rotumans.
American Anthropologist
66(2): 266-283.
Howard, Alan and Scott,
Robert A. 1965. A
proposed framework for
the analysis of stress in
the human organism.
Behavioral Science
10:
141-160.
— 1965/66. Cultural values
and attitudes toward death.
Journal of Existentialism
6: 161-174.
Huggins, Martha K. 2004.
Torture 101: What
sociology can teach us.
Anthropology News
45(6):
12-13.
Human Ecology Fund [HEF]
1963.
Report
. Forest
Hills, NY: Society for the
Investigation of Human
Ecology.
Intelligence Science Board
[ISB] (ed.) 2006.
Educing
information
. Washington,
DC: NDIC. Accessed at:
educing.pdf
Jones, Delmos 1971. Social
responsibility and the
belief in basic research.
Current Anthropology
12(3): 347-350.
Kleinman, Steven M.
2006. KUBARK
counterintelligence
interrogation review:
Observations of an
interrogator.’ In: ISB (ed.)
Educing information
,
pp. 95-140. Washington,
DC: NDIC; accessed at:
educing.pdf
Lagouranis, Tony and
Mikaelian, Allen 2007.
Fear up harsh
. New York:
NAL Caliber.
Mackey, Chris and
Miler, Greg 2004.
The
interrogators.
New York:
Little, Brown.
Marks, John 1979.
The search
for the ‘Manchurian
candidate’
. New York:
Times Books.
McCoy, Alfred 2006.
A
question of torture
. New
York: Henry Holt.
McNamara, Laura 2007.
Culture, critique and
credibility.
Anthropology
Today
23(2): 20.
Opler, Marvin K. 1965.
Report on the First
International Congress
of Social Psychiatry in
London, England, August
17-22, 1964.
Current
Anthropology
6(3): 294.
Price, David H. 1998.
Cold War anthropology.
Identities
4(3-4): 389-430.
— 1989.
Atlas of world
cultures
. Newbury Park:
Sage [reprinted Blackburn
Press, 2004].
— 2003. Subtle means and
enticing carrots.
Critique
of Anthropology
23(4):
373-401.
Rév, Istán 2002.
The suggestion.
Representations
80: 62-98.
Roelofs, Joan 2003.
resources were available; they could have a ‘divergent
response’ in which they diverted ‘energies and resources
away from the confronting problem’, often in the form
of a withdrawal; or they could have an ‘inert response’
in which they react with paralysis and refuse to respond
(1965: 147). They concluded that the ‘assertive response’
was the only viable option for an organism responding to
externally induced stress: if these findings are transposed
onto an environment of coercive interrogation, this would
mean that co-operation was the only viable option for
interrogation subjects.
In the context of MK-Ultra’s interest in developing
effective interrogation methods, these three responses took
on other meanings. Interrogation subjects producing an
‘assertive response’ would co-operate with interrogators
and provide them with the desired information; subjects
producing a ‘divergent response’ might react to interroga-
tion by mentally drifting away from the present dilemma,
or by fruitless efforts to redirect enquiries; subjects pro-
ducing an ‘inert response’ would freeze – like the torture
machine’s victims in Kafka’s
Penal colony
.
Kubark
described how interrogators use ‘manipulated
techniques’ that are ‘still keyed to the individual but brought
to bear on himself’, creating stresses for the individual and
pushing him towards a state of ‘regression of the person-
ality to whatever earlier and weaker level is required for
the dissolution of resistance and the inculcation of depend-
ence’ (CIA 1963b: 41). In
Kubark
, successful interrogators
get interrogation subjects to view them as liberators who
will help them find a way to return to the desired state
of release: ‘[a]s regression proceeds, almost all resisters
feel the growing internal stress that results from wanting
simultaneously to conceal and to divulge… It is the busi-
ness of the interrogator to provide the right rationalization
at the right time’ (ibid.: 40-41).
Kubark
recognized that the
stress created in an interrogation environment was a useful
tool for interrogators who understood their role as helping
subjects find release from this stress.
[T]he interrogator can benefit from the subject’s anxiety. As
the interrogator becomes linked in the subject’s mind with the
reward of lessened anxiety, human contact, and meaningful
activity, and thus with providing relief for growing discomfort,
the questioner assumes a benevolent role. (ibid.: 90)
Under Howard and Scott’s learning model, the inter-
rogator’s role becomes not that of the person delivering
discomfort, but that of an individual acting as the gateway
to obtaining mastery of a problem.
Howard and Scott found that once an individual con-
quers stress through an assertive response, then ‘the state
of the organism will be superior to its state prior to the time
it was confronted with the problem, and that should the
same problem arise again (after the organism has had an
opportunity to replenish its resources) it will be dealt with
more efficiently than before’ (1965: 149). When applied
to coercive interrogations, these findings suggest that sub-
jects will learn to produce the desired information ‘more
efficiently than before’. But as
Kubark
warned, this could
also mean that an individual who endured coercive inter-
rogation but did not produce information on the first try
might well learn that he can survive without giving infor-
mation (CIA 1963b, CIA 1983).
One of
Kubark
’s techniques, called ‘Spinoza and
Mortimer Snerd’ described how interrogators could ensure
co-operation by interrogating subjects for prolonged
periods ‘about lofty topics that the source knows nothing
about’ (CIA 1963b: 75). The subject is forced to say hon-
estly s/he does not know the answers to these questions,
and some measure of stress is generated and maintained.
When the interrogator switches to known topics, the sub-
ject is given small rewards and feelings of relief emerge as
these conditions are changed. Howard and Scott’s model
was well suited to being adapted to such interrogation
methods, as release from stress was
Kubark
’s hallmark of
effective interrogation techniques.
Kubark
described how prisoners come to be ‘helplessly
dependent on their captors for the satisfaction of their many
basic needs’ and release of stress. The manual taught that:
once a true confession is obtained, the classic cautions apply.
The pressures are lifted, at least enough so that the subject
can provide counterintelligence information as accurately as
possible. In fact, the relief granted the subject at this time fits
neatly into the interrogation plan. He is told that the changed
treatment is a reward for truthfulness and as evidence that
friendly handling will continue as long as he cooperates. (CIA
ibid.: 84)
Translated into Howard and Scott’s stress model: this
subject mastered the environment by using an ‘assertive
response’ that allowed him/her to return to the desired state
of equilibrium. There remain basic problems of knowing
when a ‘true confession’ is actually a false confession
– offered simply in order to return to the desired state of
equilibrium.
This research on stress gave the CIA access to an ele-
gant cross-cultural analytical model explaining human
responses to stress. It did not matter that the model was not
produced by scholars for such ends; the CIA had its own
private uses for the work they funded. As Alan Howard
clarifies, the abuse of their work was facilitated by the
CIA’s secrecy:
I could liken our situation to the discovery of the potential of
splitting atoms for the release of massive amounts of energy.
That knowledge can be used to create energy sources to sup-
port the finest human endeavors or to make atomic bombs.
Unfortunately, such is the potential of most forms of human
knowledge; it can be used for good or evil. While there is no
simple solution to this dilemma, it is imperative that scientists
of every ilk demand transparency in the funding of research and
open access to information. The bad guys will, of course, opt
for deception whenever it suits their purposes, and we cannot
control that, but exposing such deceptions, as you have so ably
done, is vitally important. (AH to DP 6/7/07)
Unwitting past, but witless present?
Use of CIA funds to commission research covertly was
common. The Human Ecology Fund was one of many CIA
funding fronts; among the most significant exposed fronts
from this period are the Beacon Fund, the Borden Trust,
the Edsel Fund, Gotham Foundation, the Andrew Hamilton
Fund, the Kentfield Fund, the Michigan Fund and the
Price Fund, but a number of academic presses, including
Praeger Press, also served as CIA conduits (Roelofs 2003,
Saunders 1999). Given the Church Committee finding that
between 1963 and 1966, ‘CIA funding was involved in
nearly half the grants of the non-Big Three foundations
[Rockefeller, Ford, Carnegie] in the field of international
activities’, perhaps the most remarkable feature of this
HEF research is only that we can connect its CIA funding
with the project it was used for –
not
that it was financed
by CIA funds (US Senate 1976:182).
However, it does not take CIA funding for anthropologists
to produce research consumed by military and intelligence
agencies. During the 1993 American military actions in
Somalia I read a news article mentioning an ethnographic
map issued by the CIA to Army Rangers. Because of my
interest in ethnographic mapping, I wrote to the CIA’s car-
tographic section requesting a copy of this map. A CIA
staff member responded to my query, informing me that no
such map was available to the public. This CIA employee
also politely acknowledged that she was familiar with a
book I had published while a graduate student that mapped
the geographical location of about 3000 cultural groups
(Price 1989). Given the CIA’s historic role in undermining
democratic movements around the world, I was disheart-
ANTHROPOLOGY TODAY VOL 23 NO 5, OcTObeR 2007
21
[ Pobierz całość w formacie PDF ]
  • zanotowane.pl
  • doc.pisz.pl
  • pdf.pisz.pl
  • dodatni.htw.pl